Rationality Drugs

Can drugs improve your rationality?

I’m not sure, but it seems likely.

Remember the cognitive science of rationality. Often, irrationality is a result of ‘mindware gaps’ or ‘contaminated mindware’ — missing pieces of knowledge like probability theory, or wrong ideas like supernaturalism. Alas, we cannot yet put probability theory in a pill and feed it to people, nor can a pill deprogram someone from supernaturalism.

Another cause of irrationality is ‘cognitive miserliness’. We default to automatic, unconscious, inaccurate processes whenever possible. Even if we manage to override those processes with slow deliberation, we usually perform the easiest deliberation possible — deliberation with a ‘focal bias’ like confirmation bias.

What will increase the likelihood of cognitive override and decrease the effect of focal biases? First, high cognitive capabilities (IQ, working memory, etc.) make a brain able to do the computationally difficult processing required for cognitive override and avoidance of focal bias. Second, a disposition for cognitive reflectiveness make it more likely that someone will choose to use those cognitive capabilities to override automatic reasoning processes and reason with less bias.1

Thus, if drugs can increase cognitive capability or increase cognitive reflectiveness, then such drugs may be capable of increasing one’s rationality.

First: Can drugs increase cognitive capability?

Yes. Many drugs have been shown to increase cognitive capability. Here are a few of them:2

  • Modafinil improves working memory, digit span, visual pattern recognition, spatial planning, and reaction time.3

  • Because glucose is the brain’s main energy source,4 increases in glucose availability via sugar injestion should improve memory performance.5

  • Creatine improves cognitive performance.6

  • Donepezil improves memory performance, but perhaps only after taken for 21 days.7

  • Dopamine agonists like d-amphetamine, bromocriptine, and pergolide have all been been found to improve working memory and executive function,8 but perhaps only in those with poor memory performance.9

  • Guanfacine has shown mixed effects on cognition.10 Methylphenidate (Ritalin) has also shown mixed results for cognitive enhancement,11 though the most commonly reported motive for illicit use of prescription stimulants like methylphenidate is to enhance concentration and alertness for studying purposes.12

  • Piracetam is usually prescribed to deal with cognitive deficits and other problems, but also has also shown some cognitive benefits in healthy individuals.13

Second: Can drugs increase cognitive reflectiveness?

I’m not sure. I’m not yet aware of any drugs that have been shown to increase one’s cognitive reflectiveness.

So, can drugs improve your rationality? I haven’t seen any experimental studies test whether particular drugs improve performance on standard tests of rationality like the CRT. However, our understanding of how human irrationality works suggests that improvements in cognitive capability and cognitive reflectiveness (via drugs or other means) should increase one’s capacity to think and act rationally. That said, current drugs probably can’t improve rationality as much as demonstrated debiasing practices can.

Should we use drugs for cognitive enhancement? Scholars debate whether such modifications to human functioning are ethical or wise,14 but I think the simplicity of the transhumanist position is pretty compelling:

If we can make things better, then we should, like, do that.15

Notes

1 For a review, see Stanovich (2010), ch. 2.

2 For a broader overview, see de Jongh et al. (2008); Normann & Berger (2008); Sandberg (2011).

3 Muller et al. (2004); Turner et al. (2004); Gill et al. (2006); Caldwell et al. (2000); Finke et al. (2010); Repnatis et al. (2010).

4 Fox et al. (1988).

5 Foster et al. (1999); Sunram-Lea et al. (2002).

6 Rae et al. (2003); McMorris et al. (2006); Watanabe et al. (2002).

7 Gron et al. (2005).

8 D-amphetamine: Mattay et al. (2000); Mattay et al. (2003); Barch & Carter (2005). Bromocriptine: Kimberg et al. (1997); Kimberg et al. (2001); Mehta et al. (2001); Roesch-Ely et al. (2005); Gibbs & D’Esposito (2005a). Pergolide: Muller et al. (1998); Kimberg & D’Esposito (2003).

9 Kimberg et al. (1997); Mehta et al. (2001); Mattay et al. (2000); Mattay et al. (2003); Gibbs & D’Esposito (2005a, 2005b).

10 Muller et al. (2005); de Jongh et al. (2008).

11 de Jongh et al. (2008).

12 Teter et al. (2006).

13 Dimond & Brouwers (1976); Mondadori (1996).

14 Savulescu & Bostrom (2009).

15 I think I first heard Louie Helm put it this way.

References

Barch & Carter (2005). Amphetamine improves cognitive function in medicated individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research, 77: 43–58.

Caldwell, Caldwell, et al. (2000). A double-blind, placebo-controlled investigation of the efficacy of modafinil for sustaining the alertness and performance of aviators: A helicopter simulator study. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 150: 272–282.

de Jongh, Bolt, Schermer, & Olivier (2008). Botox for the brain: Enhancement of cognition, mood, and pro-social behavior and blunting of unwanted memories. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32: 760-776.

Dimond & Brouwers (1976). Increase in the power of human memory in normal man through the use of drugs. Psychopharmacology, 49: 307–309.

Finke, Dodds, et al. (2010). Effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on visual attention capacity: a TVA-based study. Psychopharmacology, 210: 317-329.

Foster, Lidder, & Sunram (1998). Glucose and memory: fractionation of enhancement effects? Psychopharmacology, 137: 259–270.

Fox, Raichle, et at. (1988). Nonoxidative glucose consumption during focal physiologic neural activity. Science, 241: 462–464.

Gibbs & D’Esposito (2005a). Individual capacity differences predict working memory performance and prefrontal activity following dopamine receptor stimulation. Cognitive & Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 5: 212–221.

Gibbs & D’Esposito (2005b). A functional MRI study of the effects of bromocriptine, a dopamine receptor agonist, on component processes of working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 180: 644–653.

Gill, Haerich, et al. (2006). Cognitive performance following modafinil versus placebo in sleep-deprived emergency physicians: A double-blind randomized crossover study. Academic Emergency Medicine, 13: 158–165.

Gron, Kirstein, et al. (2005). Cholinergic enhancement of episodic memory in healthy young adults. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 182: 170–179.

Kimberg, D’Esposito, & Farah (1997). Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. Neuroreport, 8: 3581–3585.

Kimberg, Aguirre, et al. (2001). Cortical effects of bromocriptine, a D-2 dopamine receptor agonist, in human subjects, revealed by fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 12: 246–257.

Kimberg & D’Esposito (2003). Cognitive effects of the dopamine receptor agonist pergolide. Neuropsychologia, 41: 1020–1027.

Mattay, Callicott, et al. (2000). Effects of dextroamphetamine on cognitive performance and cortical activation. Neuroimage, 12: 268–275.

Mattay, Goldberg, et al. (2003). Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100: 6186–6191.

McMorris, Harris, et al. (2006). Effect of creatine supplementation and sleep deprivation, with mild exercise, on cognitive and psychomotor performance, mood state, and plasma concentrations of catecholamines and cortisol. Psychopharmacology, 185: 93–103.

Mehta, Swainson, et al. (2001). Improved short-term spatial memory but impaired reversal learning following the dopamine D(2) agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 159: 10–20.

Mondadori (1996). Nootropics: Preclinical results in the light of clinical effects; comparison with tacrine. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology, 10: 357–370.

Muller, von Cramon, & Pollmann (1998). D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 18: 2720–2728.

Muller, Steffenhagen, et al. (2004). Effects of modafinil on working memory processes in humans. Psychopharmacology, 177: 161–169.

Muller, Clark, et al. (2005). Lack of effects of guanfacine on executive and memory functions in healthy male volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 182: 205–213.

Normann & Berger (2008). Neuroenhancement: status quo and perspectives. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258 Supplement 5: 110-114.

Rae, Digney, et al. (2003). Oral creatine monohydrate supplementation improves brain performance: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biotogical Sciences, 270: 2147–2150.

Repantis, Schlattmann, et al. (2010). Modafinil and methylphenidate for neuroenhancement in healthy individuals: A systematic review. Pharmacological Research, 62: 187-206.

Roesch-Ely, Scheffel, et al. (2005). Differential dopaminergic modulation of executive control in healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 178: 420–430.

Sandberg (2011). Cognition enhancement: Upgrading the brain. In Savulescu, ter Meulen, & Kahane (eds.), Enhancing Human Capacities. Wiley-Blackwell.

Savulescu & Bostrom (2009). Human Enhancement. Oxford University Press.

Stanovich (2010). Rationality and the Reflective Mind. Oxford University Press.

Sunram-Lea, Foster, et al. (2002). Investigation into the significance of task difficulty and divided allocation of resources on the glucose memory facilitation effect. Psychopharmacology, 160: 387–397.

Teter, Robbins, et al. (2003). Cognitive enhancing effects of modafinil in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 165: 260–269.

Turner, Clark, Dowson, Robbins, & Sahakian (2004). Modafinil improves cognition and response inhibition in adult attention-deficit/​hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 55: 1031-1040.

Watanabe, Kato, et al. (2002). Effects of creatine on mental fatigue and cerebral hemoglobin oxygenation. Neuroscience Research, 42: 279–285.